Effect of diurnal changes on the auditory working memory in individuals with normal hearing

Authors

  • Praveen Prakash Department of Audiology All India Institute of Speech and Hearing Mysuru, Naimisham Campus, Road No.3 TK Layout, Manasagangotri, Mysuru Karnataka 570006, India
  • Chandana Shivaiah Department of Audiology All India Institute of Speech and Hearing Mysuru, Naimisham Campus, Road No.3 TK Layout, Manasagangotri, Mysuru Karnataka 570006, India
  • Abishek Umashankar Department of Audiology All India Institute of Speech and Hearing Mysuru, Naimisham Campus, Road No.3 TK Layout, Manasagangotri, Mysuru Karnataka 570006, India
  • Prashanth Prabhu Department of Audiology All India Institute of Speech and Hearing Mysuru, Naimisham Campus, Road No.3 TK Layout, Manasagangotri, Mysuru Karnataka 570006, India

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.31117/neuroscirn.v4i4.110

Keywords:

working memory, diurnal effects, higher-order functions

Abstract

The human circadian rhythmicity is an internal biological clock mechanism that enables them to effectively perform tasks during a particular time of the day, due to which they exhibit diurnal effects. The morningness-eveningness questionnaire classifies individuals as definitely morning, moderately morning, intermediate, moderately evening, and definitely evening type individuals based on their active performance during different times of the day. Literature show variations in visual, memory, audition, and other cognitive tasks throughout the day in every individual. The current study aimed to document the diurnal effects on auditory working memory, a phenomenon crucial for learning and academic outcomes and holds its role in various clinical and research fields. Thirty-two participants were enrolled (21 females and 11 males) and were classified based on the morningness-eveningness questionnaire. The Auditory Working Memory tests were carried out during the morning and evening for all the participants. Based on a parametric paired t-test, results reveal no significant differences between morning time and evening time across moderately morning, intermediate, and moderately evening groups implying that working memory is a higher-order function that shows no or negligible diurnal effects, unlike other lower-order functions like temporal processing of auditory signals.

References

Albouy, P., Mattout, J., Bouet, R., Maby, E., Sanchez, G., Aguera, P. E., Daligault, S., Delpuech, C., Bertrand, O., Caclin, A., & Tillmann, B. (2013). Impaired pitch perception and memory in congenital amusia: The deficit starts in the auditory cortex. Brain, 136(5), 1639–1661. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt082

Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 20–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003

Alloway, T. P., Rajendran, G., & Archibald, L. M. D. (2009). Working memory in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(4), 372–382. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219409335214

Anderson, M. J., Petros, T. V., Beckwith, B. E., Mitchell, W. W., & Fritz, S. (1991). Individual Differences in the Effect of Time of Day on Long-Term Memory Access. The American Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 241. https://doi.org/10.2307/1423157

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory, 8(C), 47–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1

Baddeley, A. D., Hatter, J. E., Scott, D., & Snashall, A. (1970). Memory and Time of Day. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22(4), 605–609. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640747008401939

Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 575–589. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(75)80045-4

Berson, D. M., Dunn, F. A., & Takao, M. (2002). Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science, 295(5557), 1070–1073. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1067262

Carhart, R., & Jerger, J. F. (1959). Preferred method for clinical determination of pure-tone thresholds. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 24(4), 330–345. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.2404.330

Cowan, N., Wood, N. L., Nugent, L. D., & Treisman, M. (1997). There Are Two Word-Length Effects in Verbal Short-Term Memory: Opposed Effects of Duration and Complexity. Psychological Science, 8(4), 290–295. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00440.x

Duffy, J. F., & Wright, K. P. (2005). Entrainment of the human circadian system by light. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 20(4), 326–338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730405277983

Ezzatian, P., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., & Schneider, B. A. (2010). Do Circadian Rhythms Affect Adult Age-Related Differences in Auditory Performance? Canadian Journal on Aging / La Revue Canadienne Du Vieillissement, 29(2), 215–221. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0714980810000139

Frey, F. (2017). SPSS (Software) . The International Encyclopedia of Communication Research Methods, 1–2. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118901731.iecrm0237

Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1976). A self assessment questionnaire to determine Morningness Eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4(2), 97–110. http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/1027738%0Apapers3://publication/uuid/F0AE4A5C-52EA-432A-AFDB-2C905277F3E1

Huang, J., Katsuura, T., Shimomura, Y., & Iwanaga, K. (2005). Diurnal Changes of Human Cognitive Function in Response to Sound Stimuli of Different Frequencies for Evening-Type Subjects. Journal of the Human-Environment System, 8(1), 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1618/jhes.8.7

Katzenberg, D., Young, T., Finn, L., Lin, L., King, D. P., Takahashi, J. S., & Mignot, E. (1998). A CLOCK polymorphism associated with human diurnal preference. Sleep, 21(6), 569–576. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/21.6.569

Kerkhof, G. A., Korving, H. J., Geest, H. M. M. W. v.d., & Rietveld, W. J. (1980). Diurnal differences between morning-type and evening-type subjects in self-rated alertness, body temperature and the visual and auditory evoked potential. Neuroscience Letters, 16(1), 11–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(80)90093-2

Kerkhof, G. A. (1982). Event‐Related Potentials and Auditory Signal Detection: Their Diurnal Variation for Morning‐Type and Evening‐Type Subjects. Psychophysiology, 19(1), 94–103. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1982.tb02607.x

Kerkhof, G. A. (1985). Inter-individual differences in the human circadian system: A review. Biological Psychology, 20(2), 83–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(85)90019-5

Kumar, S., Bonnici, H. M., Teki, S., Agus, T. R., Pressnitzer, D., Maguire, E. A., & Griffiths, T. D. (2014). Representations of specific acoustic patterns in the auditory cortex and hippocampus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1791). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1000

Kumar, S., Joseph, S., Gander, P. E., Barascud, N., Halpern, A. R., & Griffiths, T. D. (2016). A brain system for auditory working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(16), 4492–4505. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4341-14.2016

MacKenzie, C. (2000). Time of Day Effects on the Performance on the Screening Test of Auditory Processing. Theses and Dissertations. https://commons.und.edu/theses/973

Nagaraj, N. K., & Magimairaj, B. M. (2020). Auditory processing in children: Role of working memory and lexical ability in auditory closure. PLOS ONE, 15(11), e0240534. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240534

Nikhil, J., Megha, K. N., & Prabhu, P. (2018). Diurnal changes in differential sensitivity and temporal resolution in morning-type and evening-type individuals with normal hearing. World Journal of Otorhinolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery, 4(4), 229–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wjorl.2017.10.001

Petros, T. V., Beckwith, B. E., & Anderson, M. (1990). Individual differences in the effects of time of day and passage difficulty on prose memory in adults. British Journal of Psychology, 81(1), 63–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02346.x

Prakash, P., Jayan, A., & Prabhu, P. (2021). Effects of diurnal changes on temporal processing in morning-type and evening-type individuals with normal hearing. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology, 278, 3073–3079. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00405-021-06605-y

Revell, V. L., & Eastman, C. I. (2005). How to trick mother nature into letting you fly around or stay up all night. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 20(4), 353–365. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730405277233

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(7), 755–789. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158

Visscher, K. M., Kaplan, E., Kahana, M. J., & Sekuler, R. (2007). Auditory short-term memory behaves like visual short-term memory. PLoS Biology, 5(3), 0662–0672. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050056

Vuontela, V., Steenari, M. R., Carlson, S., Koivisto, J., Fjällberg, M., & Aronen, E. T. (2003). Audiospatial and visuospatial working memory in 6-13 year old school children. Learning and Memory, 10(1), 74–81. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.53503

Yathiraj, A., & Vijayalakshmi, C. S. (2005). Phonemically balanced word list in Kannada: developed in department of audiology. Mysore: AIISH. http://aiish.ac.in/images/pdf/08-09aud.pdf

Downloads

Published

2021-12-08

How to Cite

Prakash, P., Shivaiah, C., Umashankar, A., & Prabhu, P. (2021). Effect of diurnal changes on the auditory working memory in individuals with normal hearing. Neuroscience Research Notes, 4(4), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.31117/neuroscirn.v4i4.110